Clarifying the Relationship Between Phonics Knowledge and Word Meanings
by Dr. Patrick Groff
No well-known or popular phonics instruction program ever has claimed that all that children need to know in order to learn to read is how to apply phonics information to identify ("decode") written words. Decoding a word by applying phonics information to it is done by producing and attaching to letters in the word their speech sound equivalents. It is vitally necessary for children learning to read to master this process, experimental evidence consistently indicates. However, decoding a word by applying phonics information to it is not sufficient for that purpose. None of the respected phonics programs has averred otherwise at any time. Aspersions that these programs do so, generally made by "Whole Language" advocates, thus must be disregarded as false.
The purpose of teaching children phonics information, and how to apply it to decode words, is to enable them to look at individual letters in a written word, and then to "sound out" each of them. Thus,
dog becomes /d/--/o/--/g/. By "blending" together these three speech sounds in a serial order, /d/--/o/--/g/, a child produces an approximate pronunciation of
dog. This aid to word recognition is all that the application of phonics information can provide. But as my experimental research indicates, it serves its purpose remarkably well. That is to say, when beginning readers obtain an approximate pronunciation of a familiar word, as the result of application of phonics rules, they then readily can infer the word's pronunciation.
In this regard, the term,
familiar word in reading material, must be singled out for special attention. These are words children know the meaning of as the result of their previous experiences. If children hear such a familiar word, e.g.,
dog, then someone reads it aloud to them, the image of a dog instantly would occur in their minds. The application of phonics information gives children a similar kind of access to familiar words that children try to read.
It is known, nevertheless, that application of phonics information does not provide children such access to words whose meanings or connotations are unfamiliar to them. For example, a child who has mastered the application of phonics rules easily can decode the word,
daft, as /d/--/a/--/f/--/t/. If the child does not know ahead of time that this word means stupid or foolish, or insane or crazy, sounding it out obviously is of no help to the child for discovering its meanings. In this regard, sounding-out the word,
run, for example, likely will result in vivid mental images of it by the child. This sounding-out doubtless will not provide the child the entire 179 meanings of
run that my unabridged dictionary says it has, however.
It becomes clear, therefore, that application of phonics knowledge is the best way to enable beginning readers to discover the pronunciation of written words that they are given to peruse. The wise primary-grade teacher makes sure that these are words familiar to children so that they can focus their mental energies on decoding them. At the same time, the astute teacher persists in expanding students' knowledge of basic word meanings and of the varied connotations of words. As soon as a word is familiar to students it may be included in their reading materials.
There are no factors more closely related to reading comprehension than phonics knowledge, and understanding of the meaning of words, say the empirical findings on reading development. Children thus should directly and systematically be taught to apply the former until spelling patterns in words become familiar to them. After this point, they can go directly from a familiar spelling pattern to its meaning, without having to sound-out the pattern. Children at this stage are released to devote their full mental energies to comprehending what they read. Their speed of reading increases significantly.
The most effective way to expand children's vocabulary knowledge, the experimental data reveal, is to teach word meanings in a direct and systematic fashion. Incidental learning of these meanings nonetheless does take place. For example, the child seeing the unfamiliar word,
daft, in a story may request help from someone as to its meaning. Or, the peculiar behavior of the "daft man" in the story being read may allow the child to infer successfully what the word means. Then, the particular connotation of a familiar word that an author intended, which of the 179 meanings of
run, for example, depends on the sentence in which run is found.
Thus, to a certain lesser degree, a child learns the meaning of a word intended by its author through the process of reading it. However, this fact does not confirm the Whole Language principle that children best learn to read "by reading," that is, that "almost all the rules, all the cues, and all the feedback [needed to learn to read] can be obtained only through the act of reading itself" (California State Department of Education,
English-Language Arts Framework for California Public Schools).